Grooming Gossip & the Evolution of Language

Why You Were Born to Gossip | Psychology Today

If evolutionary psychologist Robin Dunbar is right, language evolved for gossip. At any rate, we certainly do enough of it. Think of how many hours a day you spend in idle chit-chat, mostly talking about who did what to whom.

Gossip is exciting, even when the events discussed are mundane, and swapping stories during talk-in-interaction is probably - the most important tool we have in building social relationships - Moreover, 

since gossip provides us with useful information about people in our  social network, the holder of a juicy tidbit gains social capital by selectively sharing it with others.

It’s hard to imagine two humans building a relationship without ever speaking to each other. And yet our chimpanzee cousins build complex social networks without uttering a word. Chimpanzee vocalizations clearly play a social role, particularly  laughter. While vocalizations help cement group cohesion, one-on-one relationships are built through other means.

In the primate world - friendships are maintained through the practice of picking fleas and dirt from the fur of other members in the group - Known as social grooming, it’s quite literally a “you scratch my back and I’ll scratch yours” kind of relationship. Although grooming does serve a hygienic purpose, cleaning the fur and skin of insects and debris, it also solidifies friendships.

Humans also engage in social grooming—doing each other’s hair, primping each other’s clothes. But according to Dunbar, we’ve found an easier, more effective way of building and maintaining relationships - idle chit-chat. In other words, 

for us - gossip serves the same purpose of - social network building as does mutual grooming for chimpanzees...

https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/talking-apes/201502/why-you-were-born-gossip


A Couple of Chimps Sitting Around Talking

Robin Dunbar, a professor of psychology at the University of Liverpool, offers an evolutionary explanation for another of our guilty little pleasures: gossip. No matter how lofty our intellectual aspirations, we are chatterers and snoops every one of us, greedy for stories about friends, relations, rivals, fictional characters, O. J. Simpson.

As Mr. Dunbar notes at the outset of 'Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language,' his systematic eavesdropping has shown that people devote about two-thirds of any conversation to gossip. It doesn't matter if they're in a neighborhood bar or the cafeteria of a great university, if they're men or women, clerks or corporate executives -- people preferentially talk about people. Who is doing what to whom, and do the spouses know? Is he as boring as he looks? Do you think she's getting fat? 

A heated exchange about politics, philosophy or the latest soccer match might occasionally intervene, but within five minutes, the author says, the discussion returns to "the natural rhythms of social life."

Mr. Dunbar argues that - our appetite for gossip - is not only natural but the source of what makes us human. Without it, he says, we might never have bothered to learn how to talk. Forget classic theories that language evolved so that hunters could coordinate a mastadon chase or gatherers describe the distribution of edible plants. By Dunbar's reckoning - language - our noblest skill, the clay of Shakespeare and Sappho - arose in the service of dish.

At the heart of this fresh and witty book is the thesis that; 

Gossip is the human version of primate grooming. 

Apes and monkeys spend many hours stroking one another's fur, the author explains, and they're not doing it for the sake of public hygiene. Long after a chimpanzee or gorilla has picked out every last flea and tick from a fellow's coat it will continue to pet, scratch, nibble and pinch. It will groom until the object of its attentions is practically falling over with drunken pleasure. 

That primates devote effort to grooming that in theory would be more productively spent foraging or mating means the practice must be essential. And so it appears that grooming is the key to primate group life. 

Most species of apes and monkeys live in groups, the better to defend territory, hunt, watch for predators and protect the young. Yet as any high school student knows, social life offers its own pitfalls. Snobs snub you, bullies push you around. Primates cope with the crowd by grooming. Whether to cement family ties, solicit new allies or apologize after a quarrel, they groom. Without constant mutual stroking, the primate social pact might soon dissolve.

But there's one big drawback to grooming, Mr. Dunbar says. It takes too much time. As the social lives of early hominids grew increasingly complex and their group size expanded, they could no longer afford to keep in touch, literally, with every last kin and comrade.

They needed a more efficient means of socializing. Hominids already knew how to make noises; why not organize those noises into recognizable patterns? 

As a social device, speech offers numerous advantages over grooming; 

  • You can speak to several individuals simultaneously, and
  • You can convey information over a wider network of individuals 
  • Speaking takes less time than grooming, and
  • You can do it while engaged in another task, like gathering food. 
In other words, talk is cheap -- and so, perhaps, were the first things humans used it to say. 
The putative link between gossip and grooming helps explain why gossip feels good enough to call "juicy." A chimpanzee being groomed is a creature awash in opiates, the body's natural tranquilizers. Assuming that gossip subserves the same neural structures as grooming does, it too may stimulate opiate production.

To buttress his argument that language arose for the sake of socializing, Dunbar offers comparative data between the brain size of different primates and the size of the groups in which they live. He has found a tidy trend suggesting that;

as the social group gets larger, so too does the volume of neocortex, the newest and so-called ''thinking'' part of the brain. 

The human neocortex is four times bigger than that of our closest relative, the chimpanzee. Plugging the data into his chart, 

Dunbar predicted that - the average human group should number about 150. And that figure, he found, is meaningful on many counts. 

  • Modern hunter-gatherer tribes... live in groups of about 150.
  • Church congregations,
  • Military companies,
  • Divisions of corporations,
  • Hutterite villages,
  • The number of acquaintances that the average person knows, 

All are close to the magic sum of 150. The size seems to fit well with the human psyche, offering both interest and intimacy.

Mr. Dunbar proposes that our forebears at some point began expanding their group size beyond chimplike dimensions to cope with changing environmental conditions and increased competitive pressures. 

As group size expanded, the need for a grooming substitute arose, and language proved the solution. 

When language arose neither the author nor anybody else can say for sure, but Mr. Dunbar suggests that the emergence of Homo sapiens, about 500,000 years ago, was marked by the appearance of language.

''Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language'' is in many ways a wonderful book, and its ideas deserve an airing. Mr. Dunbar is a clear thinker and a polymath, marshaling evidence for his thesis from such varied fields as primatology, linguistics, anthropology and genetics.

https://archive.nytimes.com/www.nytimes.com/books/97/03/09/reviews/


Wikipedia - Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language

Dunbar argues that gossip does for group-living humans what manual grooming does for other primates—it allows individuals to service their relationships and thus maintain their alliances on the basis of the principle: if you scratch my back, I'll scratch yours. Dunbar argues that as humans began living in increasingly larger social groups, the task of manually grooming all one's friends and acquaintances became so time-consuming as to be unaffordable. In response to this problem, Dunbar argues that humans invented 'a cheap and ultra-efficient form of grooming'—vocal grooming. To keep allies happy, one now needs only to 'groom' them with low-cost vocal sounds, servicing multiple allies simultaneously while keeping both hands free for other tasks. Vocal grooming then evolved gradually into vocal language—initially in the form of 'gossip'. Dunbar's hypothesis seems to be supported by the fact that the structure of language shows adaptations to the function of narration in general...

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Grooming,_Gossip_and_the_Evolution_of_Language


Book Review: Decoding Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior

Decoding Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior is a review and analysis of Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language (and the human brain). Having been immersed in experiential social media since 2006, I’ve increasingly turned to evolutionary psychology to understand human behavior at a much deeper level: by studying primates, I get much closer to the root causes of human behavior, and this book was my introduction to the field.

Inside Human OS—The Roots of Facebook Behavior Revealed by Primate Professor

Digital social networks give their members front row seats in many aspects of human dramas, but few companies or individuals have the understanding of human behavior to appreciate fully what they are seeing. Many executives of commercial and government enterprises perceive “social” behavior as frivolous and discourage employees’ activity in social networks. This exceptional book shows that the Industrial Economy idea of the separation of “work” and “social” is dangerously out of place in the Knowledge Economy, in which collaboration among people produces the lion’s share of business value. To succeed in the Knowledge Economy, leaders need to appreciate the importance of social activity in collaboration and productivity, and how digital social networks can increase productivity. In this review, I will try to do the book justice, but I will also attempt to show how its ideas apply to digital social networks, collaboration and productivity.

To use a technical metaphor, Windows has its DOS, and Mac OS X has its UNIX. In fact, Windows and Mac OS X are just interfaces that come between the core engine (DOS and UNIX) of the computer and the user, so s/he doesn’t have to have technical knowledge to run the machine. However, at critical moments, it can be very advantageous to understand certain aspects of the core operating systems. Since understanding human behavior is critical to success in virtually all human endeavors, it might be useful to understand what I’ll term as “Human OS.” This enthralling book gets way under the covers on “social” network behavior and puts it all into perspective. As such, readers come to appreciate how and why people behave the way they do.

Book Overview

Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language, by Robin Dunbar, is one of the most relevant and fascinating books I have read this decade. It reveals “Human OS” (human operating system) in an insightful, engaging way that Dunbar backs with extensive primary and secondary research into primate behavior. I have a very practical interest in Human OS: if digital social networks like LinkedIn, Facebook, Orkut and YouTube diminish the transaction costs of certain human interactions, logic holds that tremendous insight into motivations and likely uses of the networks would be gleaned by understanding Human OS. For people who want to pop the bonnet and delve into human motivations within group dynamics in digital social networks, Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language will prove an invaluable guidebook. A tremendous plus: the book is as readable as it is revealing.

As a pioneer in using digital social networks for organizational innovation, I constantly see references to “Dunbar’s Number,” which holds that humans have a practical limit to the number of acquaintances they may have in the traditional sense. Grooming, Gossip.. puts it into a rich context. But there is far more. Grooming and gossiping are core processes in Human OS, and social networks enable them in ways that confound business executives. Dunbar shows conclusively that, on average, two thirds of all human communication is “social”: talking about others and ourselves. Digital social networks are disconcerting to many because they reveal this. The most hits, the most popular videos and blog posts are often idiotic from a “serious” business point of view. Grooming, Gossip.. reveals why this makes sense, and it is a shortcut to making sense of Human OS, taking it seriously and loving it. The people and organizations that are willing to engage Human OS will succeed far more.

As in all Global Human Capital book reviews, I will summarize and outline each chapter before drawing my Analysis and Conclusions below. Note that I only give the most important books this detailed treatment. My hope is that you will appreciate the review and buy the book because it makes Human OS so accessible, fascinating and useful.

Chapter One: Talking Heads

In this introductory chapter, Dunbar connects several branches of science in the service of understanding why humans have language. By mashing up evolutionary biology, anthropology, social behavior and linguistics, he leads us down a path that does not presuppose that we should have language. There should be undeniable practical reasons that we have language and large brains.

  • Introduces “grooming” and gestures and their importance to primates in the context of social groups.
  • Tantalizing facts about the brain: it consumes 20% of our total energy, while it accounts for 2% of our body weight. Why do we have such an expensive piece of equipment?
  • Although dolphins have roughly the same brain/body ratio, they do not come close to humans in their language. Why?
  • About two thirds of all human language is social in nature: who does what with whom, why it’s important or not. Dunbar shares studies that show this ratio holds true in the boardroom, on the engineering team and university professors’ lounges. He also cites publishing, newspapers.
  • Language and linguistics have been traditionally viewed as “social” sciences, so biologists and evolutionologists stayed away; Dunbar will weave these together.

“The story is a magical mystery tour that will take us bounding from one unexpected corner of our biology to another, from history to hormones, from the very public behavior of monkeys and apes to the moments of greatest human intimacy.”

Chapter Two: Into the Social Whirl

Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior: Jane GoodallChapter Two is a crash course in the social group dynamics and economics of primates because Dunbar wants to take us on the level of Human OS, and we are primates. More depth on the role of grooming and social status.

  • Our primate lineage, and how surprisingly short it is: only about 350,000 people separate us all from our “ancestral eve.” Another 350,000, and we are back to the ancestor we share with today’s “common chimpanzee.”
  • Some primate economics: finding food, human biology, how and why we were incented to grow to our body size and live in groups to reduce the risk of being killed by predators.
  • How being social is hard-wired into our DNA.
  • The two poles: we need the group to survive, but overcrowding compels us to seek the “sanity of a solitary life.” It turns out that we manage this dichotomy by creating partnerships, coalitions and cliques.
  • How alliances work: Dunbar shares his observations of primates in such a way that it’s easy to see the applicability to human behavior. Windows into primates’ calculus when building alliances: how primates infer and predict others’ behavior in certain situations by observing their social behavior. Quote from Chimpanzee Politics. Grooming’s role in compromises and reconciliations between alliance partners.
  • The role of grooming in building and maintaining primates’ alliances.
  • The link to Darwinian evolutionary theory: the ultimate pattern of “meritocracy”: reproductive success by successful adaptation to changing circumstances.

“Sociality is at the very core of primate existence; it is their principal evolutionary strategy, the thing that marks them out as different from all other species. It is a very special kind of sociality, for it is based on intense bonds between group members, with kinship often providing a platform for these relationships.”

Chapter Three: The Importance of Being Earnest

Dunbar covers a lot of ground here, everything from deception within a group to grooming’s physiological effects on us to why it is so effective for maintaining alliances.

  • In primates, how much time a pair spend grooming each other is proportional to each partner’s willingness to defend the other. Grooming is a mutually exclusive activity, and everyone sees who is grooming whom.
  • It also turns out that grooming triggers our brains to produce endorphins (we get high); he references some interesting research to confirm the point. Astonishing fact: many primates spend roughly 20% of their time on grooming, which indicates that it plays an enormous role in their (our) well-being. The bigger the network, the more grooming required.
  • More on the economics of group size and interaction, and harassment’s impact on reproduction. You guessed it: low-ranking females, because they are harassed by the majority of the group, experience temporary infertility due to the stress harassment produces. Mental and social intimidation are more lethal than physical abuse because the victim uses the stimulus and takes it against herself. Fertility is also reduced in males under stress. The net-net here is, large groups mean more harassment even while they afford more protection against predators. It turns out that alliances are how group members diminish harassment.
  • Freeloading is another problem for large groups: freeloaders take something and promise to return the favor later but never do. Within large groups, they can trick and move on before they are found out. In this case, grooming is the cost of entry for alliances: by imposing a high cost in a visible behavior, freeloading is reduced.
  • Dunbar closes by citing research that shows that many species have sophisticated communication that approaches rudimentary language (for example, specific sounds identify specific predators).

“The solution of choice is, it seems, to form coalitions (which) allow you to defuse the opposition (harassment)… Forming a coalition with someone else allows you to act in mutual defence, so reducing the frequency and intensity of harassment without driving your harassers (completely) away.”

Chapter Four: Of Brains and Groups and Evolution

Begins with an irresistible question: why do monkeys have big brains?

  • Research on brain/body mass and Dunbar’s breakthrough, showing that neocortex size and group size have a causality. The idea: our brains are bigger because we have been forced to be social as an adaptive mechanism. Moreover, the Machiavellian hypothesis suggests primates don’t only need to remember how each member of the group relates to other members, they need to anticipate behavior given (often) hypothetical situations because they are in competition with each other, and life is about juggling goals of oneself versus others. They need to maintain a balance between numerous conflicting interests. This is a strong motor for intellectual development.
  • Dunbar’s number, which holds that the optimal group size for humans is 150, as a function of the human brain’s capacity. Interesting property: in a group, it’s the average size of four generations of an ancestral couple’s offspring, using data from hunter-gatherer societies, using hunter-gatherer family sizes.
  • Introduces the dilemma: bigger groups require more grooming to maintain the group’s structure, but there’s not enough time in the day to do it physically. This leads to the question: could language serve as a kind of vocal grooming?

Homo sapiens first appeared as long as 400,000 years ago. Throughout that long time-span, right up until the appearance of agriculture a mere 10,000 years ago, we lived as hunter-gatherers in small bands wandering through the woodlands in search of game.”

“In a nutshell, I am suggesting that language evolved to allow us to gossip.” (This drove the need for infrastructure, i.e., our brain and body size.)

Chapter Five: The Ghost in the Machine

This is all about Theory of Mind (ToM) and how the brain works. ToM holds that a (person) recognizes that someone else can have different thoughts and beliefs from his own. This gets into trust and deception.

  • Theory of Mind (ToM) applied to primates. No, humans are by no means alone here. Dunbar shares numerous hilarious and instructive examples of chimpanzees and apes that anticipate each other’s behavior in the service of competition and trickery.
  • Think of the last time you observed humans observing chimpanzees, orangutans or gorillas at the zoo; there is a marvelous fascination and a connection. Grooming, Gossip.. taps into that marvel throughout. It’s filled with hilarious anecdotes of antics of our primate cousins, but Dunbar skillfully remains on task to drive the point home.
  • How and when human children develop ToM.
  • The cases of autism and Asperger’s to illustrate human development.
  • The root of abstract thought and the ability to understand others.

“… the ability to use subtle social strategies and to exploit loopholes in the social context depends on how much computing power you have available in your brain.”

Chapter Six: Up Through the Mists of Time

Chapter Six is spellbinding. It returns to the savannah some five million years ago and offers a more complete pass at human evolution, from the perspective of the brain (and by extension, the body): group economics, grooming and language. Dunbar cites numerous studies to make his points.

  • The brain demands too much energy from the body: how did we accommodate the energy hungry brain (20% all energy)? There were very few choices: vital organs were, well, vital, so they would not give energy to the brain. The most expedient deal evolution could cut was with the gut (stomach and intestines). In fact, humans have much smaller guts than they should, given body size. Dunbar gives us a fascinating discussion of various primates, diets and gut sizes.
  • We learn that “normal” human babies are born twelve months premature, as a function of brain size. However, humans had severe evolutionary constraints in their body and brain sizes that relatively large brains and small bodies (think the mother here). We needed large brains to manage large groups, but the large brain meant that some other part of our anatomy had to give up its demands, and that was the gut. The elephant’s brain is roughly the same size as ours; that’s what I mean by having a relatively small body. Compared to other species, our brain develops extensively after birth, and human babies are much more helpless than other species’.
  • The only way the calculus worked was by changing the diet to more energy rich food that demanded less work to process. That meant meat. Notably, we ate other humans, especially early on.
  • Interesting chart showing group size increasing during primate evolution. Surprise regarding Neanderthals.
  • Group size revisited: discussion about discussion group sizes, location and language.
  • Meat moved humans toward more monogamous relationships because males could only support one or two females in most cases. It also enabled humans to migrate.
  • Cro-Magnon grew to dominate because s/he was a hunter, moved about and had a relatively diverse network when compared to Neanderthal, who stayed closer to home and had smaller networks.

“The fate of the Neanderthals (being displaced by Cro-Magnon invaders) bears an uncanny resemblance to the fate of the American Indians and the Australian Aborigines at the hands of the later Europeans, invaders who could draw on a larger and more widely distributed political and military power base.”

Chapter Seven: First Words

Here Dunbar returns to the origins of language in more detail.

  • Some interesting anatomical details: what’s required to develop the ability to produce words? What do humans have that other primates lack?
  • Several theories of language: song, gestures, dance.
  • How and why language emerged: the hunting hypothesis.
  • Dunbar’s hypothesis: female bonding and relationships were the deciding factor.
  • Large groups demanded that humans exchange social information about others (“gossip”) that were not in their immediate sphere so that group members could anticipate their behavior in certain circumstances (“she will go to bat for me in some situations” but not others, based on her interactions with other group members). In the immediate sphere around each member, members see each other’s interactions constantly and have to infer less about each other’s behavior (they know each other better).
  • Think about this: although disparaged by many, gossip is about how other people act in discrete social situations with other people, which enables gossipers to formulate opinions about how the gossipees might act in future situations with them or people they care about. Gossip often seems ugly and small, but it is a vital tool for risk management and trust.

“… there appears to be a limit on the number of people we can bond with… (the so-called ‘sympathy group’ of 10 to 15). Managing our relationships with the outer circle of acquaintances depends much more heavily on social knowledge than on emotional empathy.”

Chapter Eight: Babel’s Legacy

Chapter Eight drills down into the development of language and dialects, as well as the practical uses for dialects.

  • The life cycles of languages and dialects: cites studies that show that most humans today come from the same stock. Tracing major human migrations and evolutions of languages.
  • The importance of kin selection: several studies that show that people live out Darwinian principles in two ways: reproducing themselves andhelping their kin reproduce. The closer the kin, the better. Hamilton’s Rule, which describes the conditions under which one person will help another.
  • Presents the observation that dialects develop quickly and continuously, and explores the reasons for this. Dialects are fast-moving trains.
  • Surprise: other animals (birds) have dialects, too, and for the same reasons.
  • The importance of dialects: they tie people to a place, time and group, and they are difficult to fake. They make it very difficult for freeloaders. They make it possible to distinguish whether a person is “one of us.” Until recently, when people were tied to a time, place and group, the incidence that they were some relation was quite high.
  • I have discovered this when learning foreign languages. When you learn and use familiar idiomatic expressions, you quickly get promoted to another level with native speakers. It shows that you have spent considerable time in familiar groups who only use idioms with each other in more intimate situations. Note, it’s not only the expressions themselves; even more important is using them in the right context (this nuance is harder to fake).
  • Therefore, language and dialect are mechanisms for risk mitigation when dealing with people. Fascinating, too, that group motivation to “serve the genes.”

“… the rate at which dialects evolve is not constant, but is directly related to population density. The higher the density of people, the faster their dialects change.”

Chapter Nine: The Little Rituals of Life

Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior: chimp group. Credit: Bob Pearce

A fascinating survey of key elements of human life from an evolutionary perspective. Humans as primates and gene-bearers. This is about the stakes of survival, punctuated by interesting studies that describe behavior.

  • Gossip: a way to flush out freeloaders and social cheats.
  • Cooperativeness within the group is the human evolutionary strategy.
  • Explores three purposes of language: keep in the loop about what friends and allies are doing, exchange of information about freeloaders and influencing what others think about us (reputation management).
  • Advertising, but from a primate point of view. Dunbar cites a study he directed with these findings: both human sexes spend as much time discussing personal relationships and others’ behaviors. Out the window that “gossip” is mostly a female activity! However, males’ conversations showed an important change. In male-only conversations, art, politics and academic topics averaged 0-5%, but when males were in mixed company, these topics increased to 15-20% of topics. Conclusion, males were showing off.
  • Further, female social conversation was dominated by networking, two thirds of their social conversation was about other people. Among males, two thirds of social conversation was about themselves. Conclusions: females are more cooperative, males more competitive because they are vying for mating opportunities.
  • The role of hunting big game: not primarily for food, but advertising one’s genes. The role of “heroic” deeds in attracting better genes for mating.
  • Non-verbal communication in primates. Eye contact.
  • Smiling and laughing: its importance in group dynamics. They also release endorphins. Different types of smiles and laughs, and who typically laughs at whose jokes and why.
  • How males and females learn accents: motivations in terms of group dynamics. The wealth of the father has traditionally had a direct correlation with the survival of the offspring. Females almost always try to marry up, so they prepare verbally for that possibility. Lower class males have fewer resources and are more dependent on their networks to provide and protect opportunity.
  • Of course, this is all changing with dual income families, and females are now looking for males who have more domestic interests.
  • Language as a tool for scaling grooming: can we groom at a distance by using language? Yes.

“By exchanging information on (freeloaders’) activities, humans are able to use language both to gain advanced warning of social cheats and to shame them into conforming to accepted social standards when they do misbehave” (a key function of gossip and social networking, where customers increasingly keep companies in line).

Chapter Ten: The Scars of Evolution

Chapter Ten discusses some implications of the book on how people live today, so it is less factual and more exploratory. In a sense, it is more directly relevant to digital social networks. Dunbar wrote Grooming, Gossip… in 1998, and his discussion of some “modern” modes of communication is on point. A quick tour through modern communication situations, in group dynamics.

  • Some observation of people speaking in groups; the magic number here is four; male and female patterns; committees, conference calls. How traditions evolved to enable people to be heard in groups.
  • Modern life, and coping with the lack of kin; people are not designed for modern life, as kin are increasingly distant. Fragmentation.
  • Lack of kinship has a negative impact on physical health; cites studies of groups in nineteenth century London and the American west that show members with no kin present are more likely to die or be killed.
  • Business networks in companies, and the Internet. Road rage and Net rage due to the lack of social cues.
  • Short story about a TV production company whose productivity fell precipitously when the company changed offices; architects had eliminated the coffee lounge, which had served as the grapevine. A nice example that shows that seemingly “idle” talk is necessary for groups to function optimally.
  • Grooming, Gossip… was written before the advent of digital social networks, so I will fuse the two together in Analysis and Conclusions.

“… social group size appears to be limited by the size of the species’ neocortex; the size of human social networks appears to be limited for similar reasons to around 150; the time devoted to social grooming .. is directly related to group size because it plays a crucial role in bonding groups; language evolved to replace social grooming because it allows us to use the time we have .. more efficiently.”

Analysis and Conclusions

Key Points

If you are interested in human behavior and motivations, I hope you can see that Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language is an immensely important and valuable book. Here I will focus the book’s implications more directly on digital social networks (hereafter “social networks”). Before delving into that, here are the critical points of the book that you need to understand to appreciate the opportunity presented by social networks.

  • Historically, social behavior is the defining element of Human OS and humans’ key evolutionary strategy: “social” is hard-wired into our DNA. Executives who dismiss “social” networks take heed: we primates are group animals, and we owe our brain and body size to our need to be social as a means to survive. “Social” things may seem inane, but get over it, it’s how we are.
  • Grooming is the core activity to managing our relationships, and social networks enable us to groom each other. Interactions in digital social networks are all about grooming, LinkedIn Recommendations, writing on Facebook Walls, tagging people in Facebook photos, these are all instances of digital grooming.
  • Gossip is mission-critical to Human OS because it is our way to assess and understand the behavior of others whom we don’t know well enough to anticipate their behavior. Therefore, it is in everyone’s best interest to gossip.
  • Social networks enable us to change some of the economics of networks because they put many of the mechanisms Dunbar describes online, enabling the digital multiplier. For a business treatment, see How Social Networks Make Markets More Efficient for Buyers and Sellers.

Applying Human OS to Digital Social Networks

  • Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior: NYC marathonSocial networks are one of the most disruptive phenomena to emerge in modern society because they raise the prospect that people can fundamentally change the number and nature of their relationships. They may prove to be a significant upgrade to Human OS. Language was a tool that enabled people to scale their grooming activities and expand their networks (we jumped to another S-curve), and social networks represent a new tool that will lead to even larger networks.
  • Social networks are a digital mirror of Human OS in action, and they expose many aspects of the full spectrum, from “flattering” to “pretty seedy” ,-). I don’t think many “serious” people would like to admit that two thirds of human interaction is gossiping, silly jokes and verbal grooming. This confronts us with who we are versus how we want to see ourselves.
  • As Clotaire Rapaille explains passionately, we make decisions with the reptilian brain and justify our decisions with the cortex (intellectual). While reading Grooming, Gossip.. I kept thinking about this; one of Rapaille’s key points is that we like to think of ourselves as rational people, but we are actually quite instinctual and emotional.
  • Although Dunbar does not discuss at length “levels” of networks within the 150 limit, he states that we use social information to infer potential behaviors of people. The further away from us, the looser the tie and the more we must infer. Social networks will enable us to have much larger networks of loose connections.
  • However, as Dunbar persuasively points out, the human brain remains the bottleneck to expanding our networks. It has a limited capacity to learn and maintain information, social information, trust and relationship about people in order to maintain much larger networks. Remember, social information is filled with nuance and complexity, so I doubt that computers will be able to help to scale Dunbar’s Number in the near term. How can a machine help individuals to ascertain whether another person is trustworthy under specific circumstances? That’s what gossip does for us.
  • That said, there is a significant opportunity to create larger networks of relationships in different categories (also see Countering Social Networks’ Unique Challenges with the Relationship Life Cycle).

Key Observations about Social Networks

LinkedInFacebookMySpaceFriendster, Orkut, XingCyworldQzone and others have certain features that seek to automate social behavior that their members want, and many executives see this proposition as silly or superfluous. The networks are in a highly competitive race to attract people with Groups, Discussion Boards/Forums, Profiles and others that enable them to have more social interactions. Dunbar’s book shows that social networks get to the core of human behavior and survival.

  • Social grooming is committing ourselves to others in certain situations by “stroking” them and letting them stroke us, usually in full view of others, who extend the scene via gossip. This releases endorphins. Grooming is so important that evolution has given us a little “kicker” to encourage the behavior.
  • Social networks scale this incredibly; if you send someone an email thanking him/her, that is a one to one communication; however, if you thank him/her by writing on his/her Wall, all your Friends and his/her Friends see it, too. Depending on the intimacy of the thank you, it can be more valuable to write on the Wall.
  • I also hope Dunbar’s work causes you to doubt that “social” behavior, especially on social networks, is inherently narcissistic, as many people have claimed. Of course, like other modes of behavior, it can be, but not necessarily.
  • For more on social networks, groups and business strategy, see Geography 3.0: What It Is and What It Means.

Sociality, Trust and Productivity

  • Grooming, Gossip.. shows that people use social interaction to determine how much and under what circumstances they can trust one another. When used correctly, social networks can lower the cost of trust.
  • Dunbar’s and Rapaille’s work suggest that personal interactions get to trust far more quickly than “business” interactions. I’ll hazard that social topics can be a useful window into someone because people may be more off-guard when telling jokes than when having a business conversation. “Social” is serious business if you want people to feel comfortable with each other to trust each other enough to collaborate.
  • Of course, gossip is all about politics, which Dunbar doesn’t cover in great detail. The gossiper is presenting the gossipee in a certain light, which may be quite distorted. It requires even more brain power to decipher and use the information gossip provides.
  • Adam Christiansen shared an interesting IBM case study in which project team members who shared social information (i.e., family photos, cricket discussions, favorite music) with each other on Beehive (a digital social network) became productive close to twice as fast as control groups that didn’t share information.
  • Grooming, Gossip.. shows why this is pervasively true with people in all cultures. When people are seemingly “goofing off” and gossiping, they are really learning where they stand with each other and other people. Huge implications for collaboration and productivity.

A New Look at Features in Social Networks

Social network platforms are designed to let us gossip and share social information about each other, and they break the mold here. Scaling social information will prove disruptive in every area of society. Grooming, Gossip.. provides the context for appreciating social networks’ transformational power.

  • Profiles enable us to create a platform for our reputations in front of millions. We present ourselves as we want to be seen. Many platforms let other people gossip about us on our profiles (i.e. the Facebook Wall).
  • Networks (of Friends, Connections…) are online and visible under certain conditions. They are explicit at an unprecedented level. Often, social information around the relationship is also viewable.
  • Updates and Blogs allow us to manage our reputations, instant by instant. Twitter, Facebook, MySpace and LinkedIn enable others to contribute by writing on Walls, commenting on LinkedIn Status Visibility, retweeting, etc. Blogs enable tremendous interactivity about expressing and reporting about everything.
  • Groups, in all social networks, enable us to create private and public groups for anything. Notably, they significantly reduce the cost of interacting around any topic.
  • Photos and videos are rich media tools for sharing grooming, gestures, activities and moments. These are great tools for social grooming and, since they are scalable, they have tremendous promise.

Book Review: Decoding Evolutionary Roots of Human Social Behavior
By Christopher Rollyson
https://rollyson.net/book-review-grooming-gossip-and-the-evolution-of-language/




Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language(1996), by Robin Dunbar

Robin Dunbar (born 1947) is a British anthropologist and evolutionary biologist specialising in primate behaviour.

His 1996 work Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language pulls together his work on the social group sizes of various primates, including humans and the correlation with neocortex size relative to body mass. Although not intended as a work of popular science, the book’s style is very accessible and it may be read by non-specialists as well as academics.

The work appears to have been a considerable influence on the science-fiction novel Evolution, by Stephen Baxter.

The following is a chapter-by-chapter summary of this work:

Chapter 1. “Talking Heads”.

Describes the experience of being groomed by a monkey from the viewpoint of one of its peers: notes the similarities with the innuendoes and subtleties of everyday social experience of humans. The social life of humans, with its petty squabbles, joys and frustrations, is not unlike that of other primates. But there is one major difference – human language.

Curiously, when humans talk, most of the conversation is social tittle-tattle. Even in common-rooms of universities, etc, conversations about cultural, political, philosophical and scientific matters account for no more than a quarter of the conversation. The Sun devotes 78% of its copy to “human interest” stories and even the Times only devotes 57% to serious matters. Our much-vaunted capacity for language seems to be mainly used for exchanging information on social matters [anybody doubting Dunbar’s assertion needs look no further than Facebook]. Why should this be so?

Chapter 2. ”Into the Social Whirl”.

The answer may lie with our primate heritage. Monkeys and apes are highly sociable, their lives revolving around the small group of individuals with whom they live. They could not exist without their friends and relations.

Our ape ancestors faced disaster 10 million years ago as the climate became dryer and colder, causing their forest habitat to retreat. Monkeys became a nuisance, able to eat unripe fruit because their stomachs contain enzymes to neutralise the tannin that they contain that would give apes and humans an upset stomach. About 7 million years ago, one population of apes found itself forced out onto the savannahs bordering the forests. Mortality would have been desperately high but those that survived did so because they were able to exploit the new conditions.

All primates have had to deal with predators ranging from various felids, canids, monkey-eating eagles and even other primates. There are two main ways of dealing with primates. One is to be larger than any likely predator; the other is to live in a large group. The latter option reduces the risk in a number of ways: more eyes to detect a marauding predator; strength in numbers – a large group can drive off or even kill a predator; finally a large number of group-members fleeing in different directions will confuse a predator, often for long enough for all to get away.

But group living has disadvantages too. Social animals have to strike a balance between the dangers of predators and the problems of social tensions. The primate solution is for small groups to form coalitions within the larger overall living group. Such alliances take many forms, depending on the overall social and sexual dynamics of the species concerned. In all cases, however, grooming is the key to maintaining these alliances.

Primate alliances are built on the ability of animals to form inferences about the suitability and reliability of potential allies, but apes and monkeys can also practice deception and manipulative behaviour. They can do this because they can calculate the effect their actions are likely to have.

Chapter 3. “The Importance of Being Ernest”.

Grooming takes up a considerable amount of a monkey’s time, typically 10% though it can be as much as 20%. Grooming apparently releases endorphins, but while this encourages animals to groom it isn’t the evolutionary reason for it. One problem among social animals is “defection”, i.e. where one fails to return a favour. A solution is to make defection expensive, and grooming requires a considerable investment of time. Building alliances therefore requires commitment and it is therefore usually better to maintain existing relationships than try to build new ones.

In addition to grooming, monkeys use vocalisation to maintain their alliances. Monkeys make contact calls when moving through dense vegetation to enable the animals to keep track of one another. But subtle differences have been found in the utterances made by vervet monkeys depending on whether they are approaching a dominant animal or a subordinate one. Other calls were given when spotting another vervet group, or when moving out into open grassland. When these calls were recorded and played back, monkeys would look up when hearing calls from animals dominant over them, but ignore those from subordinates. Vervets also make a variety of predator warning calls which depend on the type of predator spotted. Other species use contact calls to keep in touch with preferred grooming partners.

But does any species, other than humans, have language? Attempts to teach apes to use language since the 1960s have produced unconvincing results, the oft-cited cases of Washoe, Kanzi etc notwithstanding. Human communications are on another level. What are they used for and why did they evolve?

Chapter 4. “Of Brains and Groups and Evolution”.

While bigger brains generally mean a smarter animal, the rule doesn’t always hold good because the size of the animal must also be taken into account. For example, whales and elephants have larger brains than humans, but they have to deal with a far greater muscle-mass than a human brain. When the relative brain-size is computed, it can be seen that the distributions for various groups of animals lie on different plains. Dinosaurs and fish lie below birds, which in turn lie below mammals. But among the mammals, there is also a hierarchy: marsupials lie at the bottom, followed by insectivores, then ungulates, then carnivores and finally at the top, primates. Here again there are levels: the prosimians at the bottom and the monkeys and apes at the top. The human brain is about nine times the size of the brain of a typical human-sized mammal and twelve times that of a human-sized insectivore [if such a species existed].

Why is this? It could not be due to chance, because of the high energy budget of a large brain, which is 20% of total in humans despite accounting for just 2% of total body mass. 1970s theories focussed on the need for greater problem-solving abilities; for example fruit eaters need bigger brains than herbivores, because supplies of fruit are harder to find.

Finding brightly-coloured fruit does require colour vision, which in turn requires more brain-power to process the input. Primates possess superior colour vision to other mammals [but so do birds and insects]. However this theory fails to explain why not all fruit eaters have large brains.

That social complexity might be linked to primate brain size was considered but not taken seriously until 1988, when British psychologists Dick Byrne and Andrew Whiten proposed what has become known as the Machiavellian Intelligence Hypothesis. This is based on the fact that monkeys and apes are able to use very sophisticated forms of social knowledge about each other. This knowledge about how others behave is used to predict how they might behave in the future and relationships are based upon these predictions.

The main problem with the theory was that many thought it was too nebulous to test. One problem for Dunbar was confounding factors: fruit eating primates require larger territories than leaf-eaters because the fruit is more widely-spaced. But many fruit-eaters such as baboons and chimpanzees are larger than leaf-eating monkeys and live in larger groups. There are four factors – body-size, brain-size, group-size and fruit-eating. The problem was to ensure that correlation between any two of these factors was not a consequence of both being correlated for quite distinct reasons with a third.

Dunbar decided to consider not the total brain size but the neocortex, which is the “thinking” part of the brain, where consciousness arises. The neocortex comprises the “grey matter” popularly associated with intelligence and it surrounds the deeper white matter in the cerebrum. In small mammals such as rodents it is smooth, but in primates and other larger mammals it has deep grooves (sulci) and wrinkles (gyri) which increase its surface area without greatly increasing its volume.

Dunbar then correlated the size of the neocortex against group size. He chose this as a measure of social complexity because of the volume of data available from field workers on many primate species and because it is a simple numerical value rather than a subjective assessment. Also group size is a measure of social complexity – the larger the group, the more relationships there are to keep track of. Dunbar found that there was a very good fit between the data and the ratio of neocortex volume to total brain volume. The findings provided support for the Machiavellian Intelligence Hypothesis – large brains were linked to the need to hold large groups together. Dunbar was also able to find the same the same correlation between neocortex ratio and group size in non-primate mammals such as vampire bats and some of the larger carnivores.

But just how does neocortex size relate to group size? There are possibilities beyond simply keeping track of social relationships. One is that the neocortex/group size correlation is more to do with quality rather than quantity of intra-group relationships. The Machiavellian hypothesis suggests that the key is the use primates make of their knowledge of others. There are two interpretations: firstly the relationship might be with the size of coalitions primates habitually remain rather than total group size, though larger coalitions are required in larger groups. The second is that primates need to be able to balance conflicting interests – playing one off against another, keeping as many happy as possible.

Research showed that primates form “grooming cliques”, with grooming occurring outside these groups being perfunctory and lacking enthusiasm; and distress calls from non-members likely to be ignored. Grooming seemed to be the glue that held coalitions together. When data about grooming clique size was combined with the data about group size and neocortex ratio, a good fit was found. As group size increases, so larger coalitions are required for mutual protection when living in these large groups.

Since humans are primates, it should be possible to predict group size for humans. The number turns out to be approximately 150 – Dunbar’s Number, as it is now known. Modern living – with millions living in cities – makes this prediction hard to test. However when considering hunter-gatherer societies – which is how we lived in pre-agricultural times, the largest grouping is a tribe, typically numbering 1500-2000 people who all speak the same language or dialect. Within tribes, smaller groupings known as clans can sometimes be discerned. Clan size – with very little variation – averages 150.

150 turns up elsewhere – early Neolithic villages typically had a population of around that number; religious communities such as the Hutterites and the Mormons lived in groups of 150; businesses can function informally with fewer than 150 employees, but require a formal management structure when the headcount exceeds this; army companies typically number around 150 men and so on.

The number of close friends and relatives people have tends to be around 11-12 with a fair degree of consistency. This corresponds to the “grooming clique” in primate societies, suggesting a neurological basis. Finally the maximum number of faces people can put a name has been found to be around 1500-2000 – suspiciously close to the size of a tribe in traditional societies.

Returning to grooming, a problem arises in that the larger primate group size is, the larger the grooming cliques need to be, and the more time needs to be devoted to grooming. Baboons and chimps have a group size of 50-55 individuals, and the amount of time they spend grooming is close to the upper limit of time that can be so spent without making inroads into the time needed to feed etc. If humans relied on grooming, then 40% of the day would have to be devoted to this activity.

The solution, Dunbar suggests, was language, which enabled several individuals to be “groomed” at once. When it comes to social networking, language has other advantages over grooming in that detailed information can be exchanged about individuals not actually present. Language is a “cheap and ultra-efficient form of grooming”. Dunbar rejects the conventional explanation of language evolving as a means to co-ordinate activities such as hunting more efficiently and suggests instead that it evolved primarily to allow humans to exchange social gossip.

Chapter 5. “The Ghost in the Machine”. 

Language is for communication [contra Bickerton, 1990], somebody trying to influence the mind of another individual. We also consider implications of what people are saying, their body language, etc. We assume everybody behaves with conscious purpose and try to divine their intentions, often extending this to animals and even inanimate objects. Philosophers however doubted if consciousness existed outside of the human world.

Rene Descartes assumed that while humans had minds, animals – which lack language – did not and were nothing more than automatons. However in the second half of the 19th Century Darwin and his contemporaries began to reconsider the emotional and mental lives of animals. A view eventually emerged that since it was not possible to see into the minds of animals, studies should focus on their observable behaviour. The result was the psychological school known as “Behaviourism”, which held sway right up until the 1980s.

Since then, however, attention has switched to “Theory of Mind”. This means the ability to understand what another individual is thinking, to ascribe beliefs that might differ from one’s own and to believe that that individual does experience those beliefs as mental states. Furthermore ToM enables individuals to handle “orders of intentionality” or beliefs about what another believes; for example “I believe x” is first-order intentionality, “I believe that he believes X” is second-order and so on. Humans can at most handle six orders, as in the following sentence due to Dan Dennett:

“I suspect [1] that you wonder [2] whether I realise [3] how hard it is for you to be sure that you understand [4] whether I mean [5] to be saying that you can recognise [6] that I can believe [7] you want [8] me to explain that most of us can only keep track of five or six orders.”

In the 1980s it was discovered that children are not born with a theory of mind and that this does not develop until 4 – 4 ½ years old. Up until then they will fail the so-called “false belief test” which asks if the child is aware that somebody can hold a false belief. For example, the child is shown an object such as a doll or some sweets being put in a particular place in the presence of another individual called (say) Fred. Fred then leaves and the object is moved. Fred returns and the child is asked where they think Fred thinks the object is. Very young children fail the test by saying they think Fred thinks the object is in the new location. They cannot grasp that Fred doesn’t know the object has been moved and that he would assume it was still in its original location. Only older children, with ToM, pass this test. Autism is a failure to develop ToM. 

Tests have been carried out on animals to see how their ToM compares with humans and to see if they have self-awareness. An early test was the mirror test, to see if an animal could recognise that a reflection of itself in a mirror was not another individual. Chimps can readily pass such tests, other great apes appear to be competent but gibbons and monkeys invariably fail, as do non-primates such as elephants and porpoises. The validity of this approach has been questioned, as animals do not encounter mirrors in the wild [though of course they might see their reflections in water].

“Tactical deception” is where one individual tries to exploit another by manipulating its knowledge of a situation. To practice tactical deception, an individual must have at least second-order intentionality. It appears to be virtually absent from the prosimians, rare in New World monkeys, but common among the socially-advanced Old World monkeys (baboons, macaques) and chimpanzees. The frequency with which species practiced tactical deception was found to show a good fit with relative neocortex size.

Dunbar reasoned that in species with a large relative neocortex size, low-ranking males should be able to use their brains to exploit loopholes in the system and mate with females, gain access to bananas, etc. Tests showed that this was the case; for example a low-ranking male would feign disinterest in a box containing a cache of bananas in the hope that a higher-ranking male who arrived on the scene shortly after would conclude that the box was locked.

Tests aimed at showing whether apes and monkeys have ToM have involved obtaining a food reward from a baited box and two human assistants, one of whom is not present when the bait is moved. The ape or monkey then had to choose which assistant they wanted to open a box. Chimps did reasonably well at the test, though less so than six-year-old children. This and other tests led researchers to conclude that chimps had limited theory of mind, but monkeys completely lacked it.

Chimps seem to be able to go to third-order intentionality on occasions, but humans can readily surpass this. Humans can envisage people and situations that do not exist in actuality – a prerequisite for producing literature. Humans are able to detach themselves from their immediate surroundings. Dunbar argues that this is a pre-requisite for both science and religion, though some of his colleagues object to the comparison! However both require one to question the world as we find it, which in turn requires third-order intentionality at minimum. 

If fourth-order intentionality and above is required for science and religion, it is no mystery why only humans have these things. But if third-order intentionality will suffice, could apes not also have science and religion? It is conceivable, but the main problem is that apes lack language and so could not transmit their ideas to their peers. 

Chapter 6. “Up through the Mists of Time”.

Five million years ago, one ape lineage seems to have made more use of the woodlands that lie beyond the edges of the shrinking forests. Animals travelling between the trees here are more exposed to the sun and Peter Wheeler has calculated that an animal walking upright under these conditions receive up to a third less heat from the sun, especially around the middle of the day. They also benefit from the slightly breezier conditions at heights above three feet. Also these upright apes could have shed body hair over the parts of their bodies not exposed to direct sunlight and improved cooling properties by sweating through the skin. A naked biped ape would expend half the amount of water on sweating compared with a furry quadruped ape.

Bipedalism begun fairly early in the hominid lineage, because “Lucy”, 3.3 my old, was already bipedal as inferred from the shape of the pelvis and the articulation of the knee and hip joints. The Laetoli footprints in Tanzania, 3.5 my old, were also made by bipeds. Quite likely these early hominids were naked although they were still more apes than humans.

In their new woodland habitat, these apes had to contend with greater predation, and in response they grew larger and increased their group size. While Lucy was a diminutive 4ft tall, the Narikitome Boy had reached a height of 5ft3 at age 11 and would have topped 6ft had he survived to adulthood. But how do we know their group size increased? The link with neocortex size offers a clue, and the link with grooming time might offer a clue as to when language evolved.

Archaeologists [in 1996] favoured a recent date of 50,000 years ago – the date of the [now abandoned] Upper Palaeolithic Revolution. Anatomists [even then] favoured a date of around 250,000 years ago, co-incident with the emergence of Homo sapiens. This was based on the fact that an asymmetry between the two halves of the brain could be detected at this point. In modern humans, the left hemisphere – where the language centres are located – is larger than the right. This, they argued, was evidence for the appearance of language.

To try to resolve the issue, Dunbar and Leslie Aiello tried to solve the group size issue and find the group size threshold that precipitated language. They reasoned it must lie somewhere between the 20% maximum grooming time for any existing primate and the predicted grooming time of 40% for humans, possibly 30%. They then discovered that in primates and carnivores [but obviously not elephants and whales], neocortex ratio is directly related to total brain size [I assume that means a linear relationship]. Given that estimates for cranial capacity were available for extinct hominids, it was possible to calculate the predicted group sizes. These remained within the ranges of existing apes at first, but rose above this with the appearance of genus Homo. 150 was reached 100,000 years ago, but by 250,000 years ago group sizes would already have reached 120-130, and grooming time would have hit a prohibitive 33-35%. But even 500,000 years ago, group sizes were at 115-120 with corresponding grooming times of 30-33%. [Rather confusingly, Dunbar then cites this time as coinciding with the emergence of Homo sapiens, having dated this event to 250,000 a short while previously. In 1996, humans from this far back were generally lumped together as archaic Homo sapiens; the term Homo heidelbergenis is now generally preferred.]

Homo erectus [then described as the predecessor to our own species] had a predicted group size of 100-120, with grooming time requirements of 25-30%. Dunbar and Aiello took the view that H. erectus lacked language. They also noted no drastic jump in grooming time at any point, which they take to mean that language emerged gradually over a long period of time. 

As group sizes increased, so vocalisation began to supplement grooming; probably this process began two million years ago, with the emergence of Homo erectus. As time passed, so the meanings conveyed by the vocalizations increased, though the purpose would have remained largely social. Humans were exploiting the greater efficiency of language as a bonding mechanism to allow themselves to live in larger groups without increasing the amount of time required for social networking. Interestingly, modern hunter-gatherers spend around 3.5 hours for women and 2.7 hours for men on social interaction in a 12 hour day, or about 25%, compared to the 20% maximum observed for other primates.

On this view, the Neanderthals must also have possessed language. They did not become extinct for lack of language, but lacked the technological and cultural sophistication of the incoming Cro-Magnons. Later, the Native Americans and Australian Aborigines suffered the same fate [basically Dunbar is advancing an Upper Palaeolithic version of the Guns, Germs and Steel argument advanced by Jared Diamond (Diamond, 1997)].

What drove the increase in human group size? Baboons can get by with group sizes of 50, why can’t we, especially as we are larger and can carry defensive weapons? Indeed, hunter-gatherers typically live in temporary camps of around 30-35 individuals. Dunbar puts forward three possibilities:

Firstly, our forbears may have occupied more open habitats than those of baboons and needed greater protection. Gelada monkeys live in very open habitats with high risk of predation. They live in groups of 100-250 [so why don’t geladas have bigger brains than humans?]. 

The second possibility is that human groups were threatened by rival human groups, and bigger group sizes were needed to fend them off.

The third possibility is that following the emergence of Homo erectus, humans became nomadic and left Africa. In unknown territory, they would have had to wonder further to find resources and they would have encountered hostile residents determined to exclude them. This does occasionally happen with hamadryas baboons. Migrants are always at a disadvantage, but one solution is to establish reciprocal alliances with neighbouring groups. This happens with the !Kung San of the Kalahari, who live in communities of 100-200 individuals, but these are dispersed into smaller family-based groups of 25-40. This is known as a fission-fusion system, because members are constantly coming and going. The same characteristic is seen with chimpanzees – the primary community is around 55, but foraging parties often only occupy 3-5 individuals. That we share this characteristic with chimpanzees suggests it emerged very early in our history.

Dunbar opts for this third theory.

The theory that language emerged to facilitate social bonding should be testable. Investigating conversations in various informal situations, Dunbar and his students discovered that conversation groups did not typically exceed four. Conversation groups start when two or three people start talking. Others join in, but once the number of participants rises above four it is difficult to hold everybody’s attention and the group tends to fissions as two distinct conversations start. At any one time, only one person will normally be speaking and the others will be listening, just as at any one time only one ape or monkey will be doing the grooming. If the speaker corresponds to the “groomer” then they are “grooming” up to three others rather than only one, meaning group size could potentially treble. The group size of 55 for chimps and baboons becomes very close to the “Dunbar Number” of 150.

The limit of four people in a conversation group arises from the distance apart people must stand if they form a circle. Once the diameter of the circle increases beyond a certain point, it is not possible for everybody to hear everybody else clearly without shouting, assuming normal background noise levels. The critical distance turns out to be two feet, and it is difficult for more than four people to all remain within this distance of each other. 

The researchers also learned that as per Dunbar’s predictions, 60-70% of the conversations concerned social topics. Politics, religion, work etc took up no more than 2-3% and even sport and leisure topics accounted for barely 10%.

Taken together, this data supports Dunbar’s theory that language evolved primarily to facilitate social bonding.

The expensive tissue hypothesis is based on the enormous energy costs of running a brain, which in humans takes up 20% percent of the total energy budget. But total energy production of mammals is a function of size and humans generate no more energy than any other mammal of that size, despite having a brain nine times larger than a typical human-sized mammal. Where does the energy to run the brain come from? Clearly it must come from savings elsewhere [unlike governments, humans have to balance the books]. In addition to the brain, the biggest energy consumers are the heart, kidneys, liver and gut – indeed these use up between them 85-90% of the body’s energy budget. The heart, kidneys and liver cannot be downsized, which leaves only the gut. With a smaller, less-efficient gut, humans have to eat high energy easy to assimilate foods. Meat is one such food, and the shift from the predominantly vegetarian diet of the australopithecines to one with higher meat content seems to have corresponded to the initial increase in brain size. Initially this would have come from scavenging, but the second phase of brain expansion, 500,000 years ago seems to correspond to the beginnings of organized hunting. Aiello and Wheeler believe that big brains only became possible with a switch to meat eating.

But big brains had another cost – the problem of getting a large brained infant down the narrow birth canal. The problem was solved by what is in effect premature birth, with a huge investment required by the parents in post-natal care. Women couldn’t do it all on their own; men had to do their bit. The tendency to male-female pair bonding was the result. Sexual dimorphism, considerable in even australopithecines, reduced. Differences in canine teeth, pronounced in cases of sexual dimorphism, almost vanished. Human males are only slightly larger than females. The implications are a shift from a strongly polygamous mating system to one that is only mildly so. Harem groups became smaller, with males having to make do with two females, and many having to get by with just the one! Provisioning more than one female would also have been expensive [a trend that has continued to the present day].

Chapter 7. “The First Words”. 

There are three competing theories for the origins of language. The first states that the earliest languages were gesture-based; the second that it arose from monkey-like vocalizations; and the third is that it arose from music.

The gestural theory arises from the fact that fine motor control used for speech and aimed throwing is generally located in the left hemisphere of the brain. In addition to fine motor control, precise control of breathing is required and for this it was necessary for the monkey’s dog-like chest to change to the flattened chest characteristic of apes. When the body’s weight is on the arms, it restricts the chests ability to expand and contract and monkeys can only breathe once with each stride.

When apes adopted a climbing lifestyle, the monkey rib-cage was a major problem. In monkeys, the shoulder blades prevent the arms swinging in a circle, which prevents them from reaching above their heads while climbing. Eventually the scapula moved round to the back of the ribcage and arm-joints became positioned on the outer edge of the chest. The flattened rib cage of the apes was the result, which had the additional benefit of freeing the constraints on breathing.

The anatomical changed permitted aimed throwing. Chimps could out-throw Olympic athletes, but their accuracy is poor because they lack the fine motor control of humans. But our fine motor control, so the theory goes, could also be used to control speech.

The problems with this theory are language involves conceptual thinking, which is quite different to aimed throwing; the complexity possible with gestures is limited; and gestures require people to be in visual contact. Communication would be impossible after dark, when one might have expected a lot of social gossip to take place.

But why did fine motor control evolve in the left hemisphere? Dunbar suggests it is because the right hemisphere was already fully utilised processing emotional information. He speculates the speech evolved in the left hemisphere because there was room there, and fine motor control evolved there later – either for the same reason or because the left side is associated with conscious thought, which is required for aimed throwing. This is the reverse of the sequence of events required by the gesture theory. The reason the majority of us are right-handed is because sensory and motor control nerves from one side of the body cross over to the other side of the brain; the left hemisphere controls the right hand side of the body, and vice-versa. 

There is a greater sensitivity to visual cues on the left hand side of the visual field. This lateralization seems to have appeared very early on and fossil trilobites tend to have more scars on the right side, suggesting pursuing predators attached more frequently from the left hand side.

Lateralization of language in the left hemisphere meant that this side became the seat of consciousness, where as emotional behaviour was seated in the right hemisphere [as Dunbar points out, this was the basis of Julian Jaynes’ theory about “bicameral minds” (Jaynes, 1976)].

It turns out that music and poetry are located in the right side. This also suggests that the theory of a musical origin for language cannot be correct.

Dunbar turns to the vocalizations of monkeys as the origins of language [a conclusion that Bickerton (1990) rejects]. He considers the predator-specific calls of vervets and also conversational patterns of gelada monkeys. The calls of these animals appear to be timed in anticipation of others rather than in simple response, just as human conversation is carried on by one individual anticipating the end of the speaker’s phrase or sentence rather than simply waiting for them to stop speaking. The vervet’s calls are an archetypal proto-language in which arbitrary sounds can be used to refer to specific objects, and overtones can be applied to increase the information content. Formalizing sound patterns to carry more information is but a small step; language is but a further small step [though Bickerton would disagree].

If it is accepted that the beginnings of language were the vocalizations of primates, there are still alternative views on the next step. One theory is that language arose out of song-and-dance rituals designed to co-ordinate the emotional states of group members [c.f. Mithen, 2006]. While Dunbar believes language arose to exchange social gossip, he considers this alternative viewpoint.

No society lacks song-and-dance, which on the face of it odd. Much has parallels with bird-song, which is used to defend territory or advertise for a mate. Maasai warriors, Maoris, All Blacks and Scots Guards use ritual song and dance (the Haka, bagpipes, etc) before going into battle. But song is also used in churches and bars in circumstances not associated with battle. Here Dunbar considers the “crowd effect” which leads to groups of people being amenable to far more extreme and intolerant views than individuals. Psychologists identified this phenomenon in the 1960s and referred to it as a “risky shift”. It led to the Crusades, Northern Ireland, Rwanda and Yugoslavia [the Nazis were of course consciously exploiting the phenomenon back in the 1930s at Nuremberg, though Dunbar does not mention this]. 

Dunbar believes that the explanation is that song and dance is an expensive activity. Deep bass tones are particularly difficult to produce. They are associated with a large and powerful body. Even among humans, there is a tendency to assume powerful, successful people to be tall. In every US presidential election since the war, the taller candidate has won. [The sequence was broken in 2000, when George W Bush (6ft) defeated Al Gore (6ft1) and subsequently held off the challenge of an even taller man, John Kerry (6ft4), in 2004. However in 2008 the taller candidate won when Barack Obama (6ft2) defeated John McCain (5ft9).] People often comment on how small the Queen is [she was positively dwarfed by Michelle Obama (5ft11)]. There is no doubt that smaller people have to work harder to get to the top and have to be fairly bloody-minded. But, as Napoleon and others have shown, it certainly can be done.

It does however appear to be almost universal that deep voices are needed to create a lasting impression. The peculiar deep voice associated with Margaret Thatcher is about half an octave below her natural voice. Thatcher’s advisers encouraged her to lower her voice after she became Tory leader in 1975.

Trying to hold together a group of 150 people is difficult even now and it must have been even harder 250,000 years ago in the woodlands of Africa. Song and dance would have had a role to play. The activity would have stimulated endorphin production. Chris Knight believes that the use of ritual to coordinate human groups by synchronising emotional states is a very ancient feature of human behaviour, coinciding with the rise of human culture and language. Ritual language would have been required to co-ordinate such activities, and this may have been the final stimulus for the evolution of language [I have to say I’m dubious; such organized rituals would surely have required the participants to already be behaviourally modern]. Dunbar is unconvinced and believes this use for language only came later. He believes song-and-dance may well have preceded language, but it was at first informal, unstructured and spontaneous, like chanting at football matches. [This probably explains why attempts by clubs and fan groups to orchestrate the atmosphere at football clubs with schemes such as “singing sections” invariably fail; also fans tend to devote at least as much time to chants abusing the referee or rival teams as they do to songs encouraging their own side.]

If language evolved to facilitate group cohesion, then who spoke first – men or women? In most primate species, females form the core of society. Males typically leave their birth group at puberty, often wondering from group to group in search of enhanced mating opportunities. Chimps [though not bonoboes] are an exception.

If early human societies were matriarchal [like the bonoboes] then language may have evolved first among females, and Chris Knight believes this was principally to help keep the men in line and ensure they invested in them and their children. This would explain that among modern humans women are generally better at verbal communication and have better social skills than men [says who?].

But in fact human society seems to be patriarchal. Evidence for this is that among most [but not all, e.g. the Karen people of Burma – see Wells (2002)] traditional societies, brides move to the village of their new husband, a system known as patrilocality. But most of these societies live in conditions where men control all the resources needed for successful reproduction, such land and hunting grounds. In more equitable societies, such as hunter-gatherers and modern industrial societies, female kinship and alliances are much stronger and matrilocality (where the man moves) may be the norm. [Does this explain the strongly sexually-differentiated culture in one particular work place with which I was familiar? On one occasion the women seemed to be in unusually high spirits all day but refused to share the reason for their good humour with any of the men. It eventually emerged that one of their numbers had just got engaged – to a man she’d already been living with for some years. At least the question of who had to move where after the marriage did not arise!]

Among Central African hunter-gatherers, Y-chromosomal genes are more widely distributed than X-chromosomal genes, which tend to be clustered, suggesting women remain close to their kin-groups, whereas men move across wide areas. A similar picture emerges from the impoverished east end of London in the 1950s, where women were dependent on the support of female kin in order to reproduce successfully and men tended to live closer to their in-laws than to their own parents.

Female kin-bonding may have been a more important force in human evolution than is often supposed [possibly because the people doing the supposing were mostly men]. The pressure to evolve language may have come from the need to form and service female alliances rather than male hunting activities.

Chapter 8. “Babel’s legacy”.

Languages change with surprising speed, the Romance languages for example having all arisen from Latin within two millennia (and having emerged as distinct languages well before that).

The Tower of Babel actually did exist and was a seven-stage ziggurat built some time during the 6th/7th century BC in what is now Iraq during the second flowering of Babylonian culture. Dunbar suggests that the Biblical story is a folk memory of a time when everybody in the world spoke the same language, but I am highly dubious. Even if we accept as late an emergence for human language as 50,000 years ago, it is inconceivable that a folk memory could persist for such a period of time. Dunbar cites the Norse legend of Ragnarok – the end of the world – as a possible folk memory. This legend tells of a world fire followed by the Fimbulvetr, a great winter lasting three years. This he equates to the folk memory of a period of global cooling. He cites a “little ice age” that affected northern Europe around 1000 BC. A more likely explanation is the eruption of Thera in the Mediterranean in 1600 BC, which would have produced a “volcanic winter” – a period of global cooling induced by dust and aerosols ejected into the upper atmosphere. These would also have led to spectacular sunsets around the world, possibly accounting for the “world fire”. At all events, the timescales are vastly different. The Babel legend is more likely a metaphor for poor project management of the kind that continues to bedevil large projects to the present day.

Dunbar then discusses the origins of the Indo-European languages, supporting Marija Gimbutas’ Kurgan Hypothesis. As I have already discussed this topic in this articleI will skip this section. Dunbar goes on to discuss attempts to reconstruct Nostratic, other linguistic superfamilies and the so-called “proto-World” [which might be ancestral to these superfamilies, but almost certainly wasn’t the first language ever spoken, since behaviourally-modern humans were probably living in Africa for at least 150,000 years before migrating to other parts of the world (McBrearty & Brooks, 2000)].

Why do languages change? This is followed by a section on language change that is also covered by my article about Indo-European origins, before asking the central question of just why does this happen? Dunbar notes that the vocalizations of other animals also show regional variations which he equates with linguistic dialects. He speculates on a common cause and believes there must be an evolutionary purpose for it. 

Most higher organisms, including humans, tend to favour kin over non-kin. Bill Hamilton showed that there are two ways of getting one’s genes into the next generation. One is to reproduce oneself and the other is to help a relative to reproduce. This principle is known as kin selection. But how does one identify kin? One way is by accent and dialect – if these are the same as your own, then in pre-industrial times they were likely to be related to you. If a group migrates, then over a few generations the language will gradually change to a distinctive dialect. The group will thus gain a distinct identity.

There is some evidence to suppose dialects evolve faster in regions of higher population density. In pre-agricultural times, the rate of language change might have been much slower.

Chapter 9. “The Little Rituals of Life”.

This chapter is concerned with sexual section, first proposed by Charles Darwin, of which the peacock’s tail is the classic example. 

Leda Cosmides was able to show that people could solve the so-called “Wason Selection” test with a far higher success rate if it was framed as a social problem rather than in terms of pure logic. She believes that humans have an inbuilt social problem-solving ability which recognises social contract situations and detects violations. Without such a mechanism, human social groups would collapse, with everybody acting in their own interest. Co-operation is essential to the survival of not only a group, but its individual members, so there is evolutionary pressure to develop such mechanisms for policing rules established for the common good. Dunbar believes that language is an important part of this system.

Language may ensure the bonding of a group in a number of ways. In addition to enabling one to keep track of friends, it can also be used to exchange information about cheats. Finally it may be used to influence what people think about us, i.e. for reputation-management. You can flatter people or be rude to them, depending on circumstances. But which function was the crucial one driven by evolutionary pressure, and which were the ones that were convenient spin-offs?

A study carried out for Dunbar showed that only 5% of conversation time is devoted to criticism and negative gossip, with a similar amount of time being spent on giving advice on how to handle social situations. Most of the time was devoted to who is doing what etc, suggesting that “policing” cheats might not have been languages primary function. While a primary function might not be one that’s needed that often, Dunbar argues it is a very expensive mechanism considering there is the far cheaper option of simply beating up the cheats! Indeed the problem of cheats is a consequence of living in large groups, which would not have happened without language in the first place.

Another study did show that while single sex groups were likely to simply exchange gossip, in mixed-sex groups there was a considerable increase in discussions about work, academic matters, politics etc with men showing the greater increase. A further study showed that of the time devoted to social gossip, men spent far more time talking about themselves than did women. 

Dunbar interpreted the results as a “vocal lek”. A lek is a display area where males gather to advertise their qualities to potential mates. It is common among antelope and some birds, such as the peacock, which do not pair for life. Each peacock will defend a small territory in an area frequented by females, displaying whenever one approaches. The females wonder from one male to another before making their choice. In the “vocal lek” the men were basically trying to impress the women.

Body language and eye contact play a key part in initiating new relationships, especially for women. This [unsurprisingly] is an ancient primate habit. In species where one male controls a harem, such as the hamadryas baboon, even if a rival male is more powerful than the harem male, he will only move in if a female’s body language indicates she is not particularly interested in her current male. Normally the females follow the male closely, but sometimes they will delay and the male stops and looks back. Rival males can detect such subtle cues.

Males also need to advertise their fitness to reproduce, and in hunter-gatherer societies, one way they may do this is by hunting large mammals. From a purely economic point of view, hunting a large antelope makes much less sense than putting out a dozen or so traps, but a successful kill with all the attendant dangers is far more impressive. Dunbar draws a comparison with chivalric tales of medieval Europe when aspiring young men had to prove their worth by performing difficult tasks such as killing dragons, rescuing sleeping beauties, etc. Risk-taking tasks have the advantage that they are difficult to cheat and thus provide a good demonstration of one’s fitness to father somebody’s children.

Geoff Miller has suggested that the evolution of the human brain was driven mainly by the demands for sexual advertising – both to catch a prospective mate and hang on to them in the face of subsequent competition. A modern male can do this by making his partner laugh – an activity which triggers endorphin release. A study has shown that women are more likely to smile or laugh than man, and more likely to do so in response to men. But unfortunately for any aspiring female comedians, men are more likely to laugh in response to other men. While this may reflect a male-dominated society, Dunbar thinks it is far more likely to be a way women access the relative merits of their current partner against other males that happen to be on the scene. A man’s ability to make a woman laugh may be as good a test of fitness as any other. 

Men and women differ considerably in the way they learn accents: men will tend to pick up the regional working-class accent; women by contrast tend to pick up a more neutral middle-class accent, the so-called Received Pronunciation or RP. One explanation is that women can improve their reproductive chances by hypergamy, or marrying up the social scale, whereas less well-off men need to be seen to belong in their environment in order to tap into the social network. Being poor with the wrong accent is a disasterous state of affairs.

But this is changing and a survey of dating agencies and personal ads show that women now want “new men” rather than rich but otherwise unreconstructed men; this is a reflection of the greater economic independence of women, and that the general levels of wealth and comfort are far higher now than they were in earlier times. With less pressure on hypergamy, Dunbar feels that women will eventually abandon RP and adopt the regional accents of men.

Sexual selection can lead to intense selection for males possessing certain traits, causing them to proliferate. This, according to Geoff Miller, was what led to the second phase of brain expansion in humans: a need to keep ones mate entertained.

Dunbar extends this theory to invoke the role of language in making people laugh, in turn triggering endorphin release.

While the ideas proposed in this chapter might not have been the main driving force behind language and large brains, they might have been valuable components that were added into the system as it developed and indeed may have driven them to levels beyond what might have occurred on the social bonding model on its own.

Chapter 10. “The Scars of Evolution”. 

This final chapter summarises the work and ends with a few cautionary tales. In particular teleconferencing seems less effective if there are more than four participants. While this limitation came about from the number of people that could stand in a circle and hear each other, it seems to have become hard wired into our brains. Finally there was the case of a medium-sized company which happened to have 150 employees. The company began to struggle when it moved into new premises without a tea room, where much of the synergy that had made the company so successful in the past had been generated.

References:

Aiello L C & Dunbar R I M (1993): “Neocortex Size, Group Size and the Evolution of Language”, Current Anthropology, Vol 34, No 2 (April 1993) pp. 184-193.

Baxter S (2002): “Evolution”, Gollancz.

Bickerton D (1990): “Language and Species”, University of Chicago Press, USA.

Diamond J (1997): “Guns, Germs and Steel”, Chatto and Windus.

Dunbar R I M (1996): “Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language”, Faber and Faber, London Boston.

Jaynes J (1976): “The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind”, Mariner Books, USA.

McBrearty S (2007): “Down with the Revolution”, in Rethinking the human revolution, McDonald Institute Monographs, University of Cambridge.

McBrearty S & Brooks A (2000): “The revolution that wasn’t: a new
interpretation of the origin of modern human behaviour”, Journal of Human Evolution (2000) 39, 453–563.

Mithen S (2005): “The Singing Neanderthal”, Weidenfeld & Nicholson.

Wells S (2002): “The Journey of Man: A Genetic Odyssey”, Penguin.

© Christopher Seddon 2009

Author: prehistorian


Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language(1996), by Robin Dunbar – The Knowledge Emporium



Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language

Robin Dunbar

Dunbar, Robin;

Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language

Harvard University Press 1997-03-10 (Hardcover, 242 pages

ISBN 9780674363342 / 0674363345 (pacificBookXch 08jun)

topics: |  evolution | language | science | anthropology

About our propensity for gossip, Dunbar asks: 

Why on earth is so much time devoted by so many to the discussion of so little? 

He then takes an evolutionary perspective and outlines how during evolution, social bonds strengthened, so that apes and monkeys share larger and more intense, close-knit social relationships than other animals. The grooming among primates is more about this social bond (and status) than about hygiene. 

As the social group became even larger (as in a human village), it was no longer possible to sustain N choose 2 grooming relations, and it became necessary to find a more convenient form of multi-person "grooming". This, Dunbar argues, is gossip. Language, he says evolved more as an enabling force for this gossip, rather than on its natural selection merits alone. 

Very well written - almost lyrical in snatches. 

Excerpts

To be groomed by a monkey is to experience primordial enotions: The initial frisson of uncertainty in an untested relationship, the gradual surrender to another's avid fingers flickering expertly across bare skin, the light pinching and picking and nibbling of flesh as hands of discovery move in surprise from one freckle to another newly discovered mole. - (opening lines, p. 1)

A human baby produces its first real words at about 18 months of age.

2 years - quite vocal; vocab of some 50 words.
3 years - 1000 - 2/3 word sentences
6 years - 13,000 words
18 yrs  - 60K  p.3

Our brain is nine times larger rel to body size than a mammal.  Only
porpoises and dolphins come close.

Most human conversations - 2/3ds is about social issues p.4 [no serious
research data].

newspapers:  L. Times: 1993 col-inches; 43% column space (850 in) goes to
	"human interest stories" - rest to serious news /reviews;
	human interest = interviews, gossip etc.
	Sun: 1063 col-in; 78% human interest, 833in (only 22% serious news)

Great Ape extinction - from 30mya to ~10mya, more apes than monkeys; but
      10mya, temp drop by 10 deg C (ocean surface) - and most ape species
      die.  [possibly because apes can't handle higher tannin diet as in
      unripe fruit]

Deception among apes

Andrew Whiten and Dick Byrne : chacma baboons in S. Africa.
Young adult female (Mel) was digging a succulent tuber out of the
ground... hard work... Young juvenile (Paul) was watching.  As soon as Mel
managed to wrench the tuber out of the ground, Paul let out an ear-splitting
scream, of the kind commonly uttered by juveniles who are being attacked by
someone bigger.  Paul's mother, who was beyond some bushes, immediately came
racing, took in the situation at a glance, and fell on the unsuspecting
Mel.  Needless to say, Mel dropped the tuber and ran off, with outraged mum
in pursuit.  Paul nonchalantly picked up the tuber and started to enjoy
lunch.  23

lg: "high dudgeon" - can dudgeon ever be "low"?
   (now used only in the phrase `in high dudgeon' - freedictionary: thesaurus)

Reconciliation after fighting among male Geladea baboons:
[After a fight, often lasting over a whole day, to decide who gets the
harem.  The females may also help decide such battles, if they prefer one
over the other. ]
   The new male approaches the defeated male in a non-aggressive, almost
   submissive way.  The defeated male is initially suspicious.  He has just
   received the beating of his life from this thug... After some false
   starts, the deal is struck surprisingly quickly; there is a simple ritual
   of reconciliation in which the old male reaches through to touch the new
   male's penis while the latter presents his rear.  Then the two males
   groom each other for the kind of enthusiasm reservved for the aftermath
   of patched-up quarrels.  27

Grooming stimulates production of endorphins - the body's natural opiates.
Opium etc. work because their chemical signature is identical to that of
the more familiar opiate drugs such as opium or its derivative, morphine.
However we don't become addicted to endogenous opiates in quite the way we
do to opium because the brain only produces natural opiates in relatively
small quantities.
     [or is it because we have no "control" over this natural production?]

Stress makes it more difficult to conceive (studies on career women) -
also men who ran > 60 miles / week had sign lower sperm densities  p.43
- this is perhaps why lower-ranked female geladas, who face some degree of
  harrassment from other higher females - 2 mild threats / day and one
  serious attack per week - they may have higher stress, which is why they
  produce fewer offspring - appox 10% less for every drop in rank.  Also
  lower ranking females have higher levels of circulating endogenous
  opiates.
[May be crucial also in explaining some aspects of behaviour - such as some
days when everything seems to go well - one wins in sports, does well at
work, etc. Low opiates? ]

Free riders in social situations
Magnus Enquist / Otto Leimar : percentage of free riders - those who promise
to return a benefit but don't - rises as size of group gets bigger.  Because
the free-rider can keep one step ahead of being "discovered".

In many species, the female will only mate w males who bring them gifts.
Making an expensive investment beforehand forces the male to invest
enough in the female, and not to abandon her. 46

Charles Hockett, 1960s: 18 features which can be taken to define true
language. 4 most important are that language is:
    - referential - sounds refer to objects in environment
    - syntactical (has grammatical structure)
    - non-iconic - most words do not resemble objects they refer to
     		   (unlike the "word" moo for example)
    - learned (as opp to being instinctive)
were proposed to distinguish human language from e.g. language of honey
bees, discovered in the 1950s by ethologist Karl von Frisch.  Through a
combination of ingenuous experiments, was able to show that the speed of
the dance indicates distance to the nectar source, while angle of the bar
of the figure of eight to the vertical indicates the compass direction rel
to the sun.
  [from http://www.cals.ncsu.edu/entomology/apiculture/PDF%20files/1.11.pdf]
     When a food source is very close to the hive (less than 50 meters), a
     forager performs a round dance. She does so by running around
     in narrow circles, suddenly reversing direction to her original
     course. She may repeat the dance several times at the same location or
     move to another location on the comb to repeat it. After the round dance
     has ended, she often distributes food to the bees following her.  A round
     dance, therefore, communicates distance ("close to the hive," in this
     example), but not direction.

     Food sources that are at intermediate distances, between 50 and 150
     meters from the hive, are described by the sickle dance. This dance is
     crescent-shaped and represents a transitional dance between the round
     dance and a waggle dance.

     The waggle dance or wag-tail dance, is performed by bees foraging at food
     sources that are more than 150 meters from the hive. This dance, unlike
     the round dance, communicates both distance and direction.  A bee that
     performs a waggle dance runs straight ahead for a short distance, returns
     in a semicircle to the starting point, runs again through the straight
     course, then makes a semicircle in the opposite direction to complete a
     full figure-eight circuit. While running the straight-line course of the
     dance, the bee's body, especially the abdomen, wags vigorously from side
     to side. This vibration of the body produces a tail-wagging motion. At
     the same time, the bee emits a buzzing sound, produced by wingbeats at a
     low audio frequency of 250 to 300 hertz or cycles per second. The buzzing
     occurs in pulsebeats of about 20 milliseconds, delivered at a rate of
     about 30 per second.

     While several variables of the waggle dance relate to distance (such as
     dance "tempo" or the duration of buzzing sounds), the duration of the
     straight-run portion of the dance, measured in seconds, is the simplest
     and most reliable indicator of distance. As the distance to the food
     source increases, the duration of the waggling portion of the dance (the
     "waggle run") also increases. The relationship is roughly linear (Figure
     3). For example, a forager that performs a waggle run that lasts 2.5
     seconds is recruiting for a food source located about 2,625 meters away.

     The orientation of the dancing bee during the straight portion of her
     waggle dance indicates the location of the food source relative to the
     sun. The angle that the bee adopts, relative to vertical, represents the
     angle to the flowers relative to the direction of the sun outside the
     hive. In other words, the dancing bee transposes the solar angle into the
     gravitational angle.

Chimpanzee language

1950s: Kellogg family and Hayes family - raised chimpanzee baby along with own
	new-born infant.   Hayes' chimp - Vicki - managed to produce ~ 5
	words, but barely audibly.  Worse, Hayes' own child picked up
	innumerable bad habits from the faster-maturing chimp, and they gave
	up. 51
late 50s: became clear that chimps did not have the vocal apparatus for
	speech.
1960s: Trixie Gardner and husband Alan: sign language to young female chimp
	Washoe.  taught ASL.  Learned ~100 signs, became the "wonder of the
	age".  But some psychologists / linguists argued that Washoe
	demonstrated nothing more than the ability to copy humans, as in the
	"Clever Hans" phenomenon.  Rarely produced sentences > 2 signs long,
	(excluding repetitions).   The supposedly novel signs she produced,
	e.g. water+bird on seeing a swan - were no more than chance
	combinations fueled by the Gardners' imaginative interpretation.
	   Gardners spent next 20 years trying to refute these charges, but
	opponents devised ever more ingenuous arguments.
1970s: ASL w gorilla Koko and orangutan Chantek
        picture-language (to avoid imitation arguments] with chimp Sarah
           - psychologist David Premack.
	Duane Rumbaugh - two chimps - Austin and Sherman - taught a computer
		keyboard language called "yerkish" (after psy pioneer Robert Yerkes)
	Sue-Savage Rumbaugh: Kanzi - taught same computer lg.  "became the
		Einstien and Shakespeare of the chimpanzee world, rolled into
		one."

Dunbar, however, isn't careful enough in detailing some of his stronger
remarks like the following -
	I think it is fair to say this research has convincingly demonstrated
	that chimps understand several important concepts, including numbers,
	how to add and subtract, the nature of basic relations (such as "is
	bigger than", "is the same as" and "is on top of"), how to ask for
	specific objects (mostly foods) or activities (a walk in the woods or
	a game of chase), and how to carry out complex instructions ("take the
	can from the fridge and put it in the next room").   Kanzi can
	translate readily from one modality to another: e.g. by pointing to
	the correct keyboard symbols for spoken Engl words heard through
	headphones.
However, even Kanzi's interactions are largely limited to asking for things he
wants, or giving the correct one-word answer to logically complex
questions. He does not engage in the kind of spontaneous, apparently
effortless chatter of the two-year-old human child learning to speak: "Look,
mummy, car!"  .... "Yes, dear, another car..." 54

Often the logic is unclear - e.g. arguing how fruit-based diet justifies the
relatively larger brain/body-size ratio in monkeys. 59
	   (see Jerison based species graph, 57)

5. Ghost in the Machine

Poem by computer program called RACTER, by NY programmer Bill Chamberlain:
	A hot and torrid bloom which
	Fans wise flames and begs to be
	Redeemed by forces black and strong
	Will now oppose my naked will
	And force me into regions of despair.

published as the slim volume The Policeman's Beard is Half Constructed
in 1984. [snippets of prose interspersed with poetry and a strange short
story about a dysfunctional group of friends sitting down for a dinner of lamb
chops. - complete text

review from [www.wopr.com/books/groom.htm]

Why is it that among all the primates, only humans have language?  Could it
be because we don't groom each other? Dunbar builds his argument touching on
the behavior of gelada baboons, Darwin's theory of evolution,
computer-generated poetry, and the significance of brain size. He begins with
the social organization of the great apes. These animals live in small groups
and maintain social cohesion through almost constant groomingactivities. 
Grooming is a way to forge alliances, establish hierarchy, offer
comfort, or make apology. Once a population expands beyond a certain number,
however, it becomes impossible for each member to maintain constant physical
contact with every other member of the group. Considering the large groups in
which human beings have found it necessary to live, Dunbar posits that we
developed language as a substitute for physical intimacy. Whether or not you
accept Dunbar's premise, his book is worth reading, if only for its animated
prose and wealth of scientific information. An obvious choice for science
buffs, Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language is a wonderful book
for anyone with an inquiring mind and an interest in what makes the world go
round.

blurb

What a big brain we have for all the small talk we make. It's an evolutionary
riddle that at long last makes sense in this intriguing book about what
gossip has done for our talkative species. Psychologist Robin Dunbar looks at
gossip as an instrument of social order and cohesion--much like the endless
grooming with which our primate cousins tend to their social relationships.

Apes and monkeys, humanity's closest kin, differ from other animals in the
intensity of these relationships. All their grooming is not so much about
hygiene as it is about cementing bonds, making friends, and influencing
fellow primates. But for early humans, grooming as a way to social success
posed a problem: given their large social groups of 150 or so, our earliest
ancestors would have had to spend almost half their time grooming one
another--an impossible burden. What Dunbar suggests--and his research,
whether in the realm of primatology or in that of gossip, confirms--is that
humans developed language to serve the same purpose, but far more
efficiently. It seems there is nothing idle about chatter, which holds
together a diverse, dynamic group--whether of hunter-gatherers, soldiers, or
workmates.

Anthropologists have long assumed that language developed in relationships
among males during activities such as hunting. Dunbar's original and
extremely interesting studies suggest otherwise: that language in fact
evolved in response to our need to keep up to date with friends and
family. We needed conversation to stay in touch, and we still need it in ways
that will not be satisfied by teleconferencing, email, or any other
communication technology. As Dunbar shows, the impersonal world of cyberspace
will not fulfill our primordial need for face-to-face contact.

From the nit-picking of chimpanzees to our chats at coffee break, from
neuroscience to paleoanthropology, Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of
Language offers a provocative view of what makes us human, what holds us
together, and what sets us apart.


to contribute some excerpts from your favourite book to book excerptise. send us a plain text file with page-numbered extracts from your favourite book. You can preface your extracts with a short review. 
email to (bookexcerptise [at] gmail [dot] com)

We reply to all feedback!


Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language
https://www.cse.iitk.ac.in/users/amit/books/dunbar-1997-grooming-gossip-evolution.html

Our Biggest Fight

Reclaiming Liberty, Humanity, and Dignity in the Digital Age The internet as we know it is broken. Here’s how we can seize back contr...